Research Paper Volume 10, Issue 7 pp 1542—1555

Locus of control as a modifiable risk factor for cognitive function in midlife

Emma Anderson1, , Alice Cochrane1, , Jean Golding2, , Stephen Nowicki3, ,

  • 1 MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit at the University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 2BN, UK
  • 2 Centre for Child and Adolescent Health, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Oakfield House, Oakfield Grove, Bristol BS8 2BN, UK
  • 3 Department of Psychology, PAIS Building, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30322, USA

Received: May 15, 2018       Accepted: June 21, 2018       Published: July 12, 2018      

https://doi.org/10.18632/aging.101490
How to Cite

Copyright: Anderson et al. This is an open‐access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 3.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Abstract

Few modifiable risk factors for cognitive decline have been identified. Despite an external locus of control (LoC) being adversely associated with many psychological and physical health outcomes, few studies have examined whether it is related to cognitive function in adulthood. In 1178 women from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, we examined whether LoC, and change in LoC over time, is associated with cognitive function in midlife. LoC was prospectively measured at mean ages 30 and 48 years using the validated Nowicki-Strickland scale. Cognitive function was examined at mean age 51 years. At both time points, greater externality was associated with lower cognitive function. For example, the group of women classified as being external at mean age 48 years had, on average, a 0.18 lower cognitive function score (95% CI: (0.11 to 0.25) than the group classified as being internal (p<0.001). Participants who changed from external to internal over time, on average, had better cognitive function than those who remained external or changed to become external. In summary, an external LoC may be detrimental to cognitive function. Thus, interventions to increase internality may help to minimise the adverse consequences on cognitive health later in life.

Introduction

Locus of control (LoC) is defined as the degree to which persons expect that outcomes are contingent on their own behaviour or personal characteristics, as opposed to a function of chance, luck, fate, under control of powerful others, or simply unpredictable [1]. Individuals with a strong internal LoC tend to believe events in their life are primarily a result their own actions. Conversely, people with a strong external LoC tend to believe events in their life are primarily a result of external factors (e.g. fate or luck). LoC is an important personality construct that has been associated with many psychological [26] and physical [7,8] health outcomes as well as health behaviours such as smoking and alcohol consumption [912]. Studies generally report better outcomes in individuals classified as being more internal than external, concluding that it is psychologically healthy to perceive that one has control over those things which one is capable of influencing. LoC is not static; studies have shown that it is malleable in response to both life events (such as trauma [13,14]) and interventions such as cognitive training [1517], thus suggesting that it is possible to improve a person’s ability to internalise, which may incur later health benefits.

Despite many psychological and physical health outcomes being studied in relation to LoC [5,7], studies examining whether LoC is related to cognitive function are scarce. Most studies to date have focused on whether cognitive training is associated with short-term [18] and long-term [16] changes in LoC (i.e. the opposite direction). Those existing studies that have examined whether LoC is associated with later cognitive function have mainly been conducted in very small sample sizes [19] or in elderly populations [20,21], whereby reverse causation (i.e. impairment of, or decline in, cognitive function affecting LoC) cannot be ruled out. Wight et al reported that an external LoC was associated with lower cognitive function in a community sample of 1835 men aged 45-59 years from the United States [22]. However, to our knowledge, no studies have examined the association in women. Given that there is evidence to suggest LoC is intervenable, it would be useful to ascertain whether individuals with an internal LoC have greater cognitive function, particularly in later life, as this may help individuals to maintain independence into old age and even potentially reduce risk of cognitive decline and dementia. Current evidence for modifiable risk factors for lower cognitive function in later life is scarce, thus, trying to identify factors on which we can intervene is crucial for prevention strategies.

In a general population sample of British women from the Avon and Longitudinal Study of Parents And Children, we aimed to examine whether (i) LoC at two time-points during adulthood (mean ages 30 and 48 years) and (ii) change in LoC across these two time-points, is prospectively associated with a composite measure of overall cognitive function at mean age 51 years, or with specific cognitive domains including long- and short-term memory, verbal fluency and intelligence, and processing speed.

Results

Descriptive characteristics of the participants

There was evidence that women included in these analyses, on average, were more likely to be white compared to non-white, to have a higher SEP and be classified as having an internal LoC compared with women excluded due to missing data (Table 1). However, the magnitude of the differences for ethnicity and SEP was small, and the mean LoC scores at both time points were similar in included versus excluded women. Correlations between the LoC scores at the two time-points was moderate (Pearson’s R=0.53). Approximately 30% of participants changed their LOC over time (12% of participants changed from external to internal and 18% changed from internal to external). Correlations between each of the cognitive function measures (Table S1) were weak to moderate: Pearson’s r ranged from 0.15 to 0.43. Logical memory and delayed logical memory were strongly correlated (r=0.83).

Table 1. Characteristics of participants.

Included participants (n=1178)Excluded participants (n=1179)
Mean (SD)/
Percentage (%)
N with available dataMean (SD)/
Percentage (%)
Exposures
Mean age 30 years
Continuous locus of control score at mean age 30 years3.30 (1.86)1,1563.87 (2.02)
Categorical locus of control at age 30 years (%)1156
Internal56.2046.19
External43.8053.81
Mean age 48 years
Continuous locus of control score at mean age 48 years3.03 (1.87)7843.58 (2.01)
Categorical locus of control at age 48 years (%)784
Internal63.0751.91
External36.9348.09
Change in locus of control score from 30 to 48 years (%)
Internal to Internal44.6552327.15
External to External25.3837.86
Internal to External11.5415.68
External to Internal18.4219.37
Outcome
Cognitive function score4.07 (0.59)1,6543.91 (0.61)
Covariables
Age at outcome assessment (years)50.96 (4.39)177950.74 (4.42)
Ethnicity (%)1558
          White98.5696.79
          Non-white1.443.21
Highest qualification (%)1022
          CSE34.3038.65
          Vocational22.1621.14
          O-level17.1514.09
          A-level19.8620.06
          Degree6.546.07
Head of household social class (%)1202
          Professional24.4517.80
          Managerial and technical48.7349.92
          Skilled non-manual20.9724.96
          Non-skilled manual4.755.74
          Partly and unskilled manual1.101.58
SD – standard deviation
Excluded participants are those who were eligible to be included in the analysis (i.e. they attended the outcome assessment clinic) but were excluded due to missing data for one LoC measure and one or more potential confounders
Participants scoring below or equal to the median (3 points) on the locus of control questionnaire were categorised as being internal. Those scoring above the median were categorised as being external.

Associations between the continuous LoC scores and overall cognitive function

Table 2 shows associations of the continuous LoC scores at the two time-points with the composite measure of cognitive function. There was evidence that a greater LoC score (i.e. greater externality) measured at both time-points (i.e. mean ages 30 and 48 years) was associated with a lower cognitive function score at mean age 51 years, with the magnitude of the association being similar for both time-points. For example, per one-unit increase in LoC score at mean age 48 years, cognitive function scores were, on average, 0.06 lower (95% CI: -0.08 to -0.04), even after adjusting for educational attainment, head of household social class, ethnicity and age at outcome assessment and the previous LoC at mean age 30 years (p<0.001).

Table 2. Associations of the continuous locus of control score at two time-points with the composite cognitive function score at mean age 51 years (N=1178).

UnadjustedAdjusted for potential confoundersAdditionally adjusted for earlier locus of control score
Beta (95% CI)PBeta (95% CI)PBeta (95% CI)P
-0.08 (-0.10 to -0.06)<0.001-0.07 (-0.08 to -0.05)<0.001--
Locus of control score at mean age 48 years-0.09 (-0.11 to -0.08)<0.001-0.06 (-0.08 to -0.04)<0.001-0.06 (-0.08 -0.04)<0.001
CI- confidence interval. Results are interpreted as the average difference in the composite cognitive function score per unit increase in the locus of control score
Adjusted for educational attainment, head of household social class, ethnicity and age at outcome assessment

Associations between categorical LoC (internal vs external) and overall cognitive function

There was evidence that participants who were categorised as having an external LoC had, on average, lower cognitive function than those who were categorised as being internal, and the magnitude of association was similar at both LoC measurement occasions (Table 3). For example, the external group at mean age 48 years had, on average, a 0.18 lower cognitive function score (95% CI: -0.25 to -0.11) than the internal group (p<0.001), even after adjusting for educational attainment, head of household social class, ethnicity and age at outcome assessment and LoC score at mean age 30 years.

Table 3. Average difference in the composite cognitive function score at mean age 51 years between participants who were categorised as having an external vs internal locus of control at two time-points (N=1178).

UnadjustedAdjusted for potential confoundersAdditionally adjusted for earlier locus of control score
Mean difference in cognitive function score (95% CI)PMean difference in cognitive function score (95% CI)PMean difference in cognitive function score (95% CI)P
External vs internal locus of control at mean age 30 years-0.28 (-0.35 to -0.22)<0.001-0.22 (-0.29 to -0.16)<0.001--
External vs internal locus of control at mean age 48 years-0.30 (-0.37 to -0.24)<0.001-0.25 (-0.32 to -0.19)<0.001-0.18 (-0.25 to -0.11)<0.001
CI- confidence interval. Results are interpreted as the average difference in the composite cognitive function score between external versus internal locus of control
Adjusted for educational attainment, head of household social class, ethnicity and age at outcome assessment

Associations between change in LoC over time and overall cognitive function

On average, individuals had the lowest cognitive function scores when they were classified as having an external LoC at both time-points (Table 4). Participants classified as having an external LoC at any of the measurement occasions had lower cognitive function scores than those who sustained an internal LoC over time. Finally, participants who changed from external to internal, on average, had higher cognitive function scores than those who remained external or changed from internal to external.

Table 4. Associations between change in locus of control from mean age 30 years to 48 years and the composite cognitive function score at mean age 51 years (N=1178).

UnadjustedAdjusted for potential confounders
Percentage (N) participantsMean difference in cognitive function score (95% CI)PMean difference in cognitive function score (95% CI)P
Change in LoC
Internal to internal (reference group)44.7 (526)----
External to external25.4 (299)-0.43 (-0.51 to -0.35)<0.001-0.35 (-0.43 to -0.27)<0.001
Internal to external11.5 (136)-0.20 (-0.31 to -0.10)<0.001-0.18 (-0.28 to -0.07)<0.01
External to internal18.4 (217)-0.18 (-0.27 to -0.09)<0.001-0.14 (-0.23 to -0.05)<0.01
CI- confidence interval. Results are interpreted as the average difference in the composite cognitive function score between each group compared to the reference group.
Adjusted for educational attainment, head of household social class, ethnicity and age at outcome assessment

Additional analyses

Participants classified as having an external LoC had, on average, lower scores for each of the six cognitive tests than those classified as internal (Table S2). The magnitude of association was very similar for each of the tests and across the two time-points that LoC was measured. On average, individuals had the lowest scores on each of the six cognitive tests when they were classified as having an external LoC at both time-points (Table S3). Participants classified as having an external LoC at any of the measurement occasions had lower cognitive scores for each of the six cognitive tests than those who sustained an internal LoC over time and again, the magnitude of association was very similar for each test. Unadjusted associations looked very similar in the larger group of participants with a measure of LoC at the first-time point only (n=2241, Table S4) compared to the results from the main analysis sample (n=1178). Results also looked very similar when additionally adjusted for exposure to psychosocial adversity in childhood as a potential confounder (n=929, Table S5).

Discussion

We found evidence that having a more internal locus of control in early and mid-adulthood is prospectively associated with better cognitive function, and importantly, that changing to have an internal LoC over the duration of the study was associated with better cognitive function than remaining external or changing to from internal to external. These findings suggest that LoC may be an amenable target for interventions aimed at increasing internality, to improve cognitive function in adulthood and consequently, help to reduce risk of ageing-related morbidity (including Alzheimer’s disease, which has been consistently associated with lower cognitive function in mid-life and mortality [23,24]). Several studies have provided evidence that LoC is modifiable, with cognitive training interventions increasing internality [1517], and Nowicki et al. [25] have identified several factors associated with changes towards both internality and externality. Our study identified a subgroup of participants (~30%) whose LoC changed over time (i.e. internal to external and vice versa), supporting the assumption that it is not a static construct and may be amenable to intervention. Given that lower cognitive function in adulthood is associated with a lack of functional dependence in old age [2628], greater risk of cognitive decline and dementia [23,24], and higher mortality rates [2932], identifying modifiable risk factors is important for informing prevention strategies.

We found evidence using both the continuous LoC score and the categorical (external vs internal) variable, suggesting that LoC across the whole spectrum is related to cognitive decline and that there is not likely to be a threshold effect whereby a certain degree of externality is detrimental for cognitive functioning. We also assessed associations of LoC with individual cognitive tests, as well as a composite score of overall cognitive function. Different cognitive tests measure different underlying systems (e.g. fluid vs crystallised intelligence) and assessing them individually may help identify possible underlying mechanisms of association. Combining measures into a composite score may, however, increase power because summing them together identifies a much higher risk group (i.e. those performing very badly on all tests), which may drive associations. In our study, higher LoC scores (i.e. greater externality) were associated with poorer performance on all individual cognitive tests, with a similar magnitude of effect for each. In addition, change in LoC overtime was similarly associated with each of the cognitive tests, suggesting that there is not one particular aspect of cognition that is largely affected by LoC, but that it influences all domains of cognitive function including memory, processing speed and verbal fluency.

Comparisons with other studies

Although there is a large body of evidence showing LoC to be associated with many psychological [2,3,5,6,33] and physical [7,8] health outcomes and health behaviours such as smoking [912] and alcohol consumption [9,17], studies examining whether LoC is related to cognitive function are scarce [22]. Several studies to date have assessed whether cognitive training is associated with short-term [18] and long-term [16] changes in LoC (i.e. examining the question in the opposite direction) and the findings suggest the relationship may be bidirectional in nature. The few existing studies that have examined whether LoC is associated with later cognitive function been conducted in very small sample sizes (N=<350) [19] or in elderly populations [20,21] where it is impossible to rule out reverse causation (i.e. age-related changes in cognitive function affecting LoC). One study previously reported that an external LoC was associated with lower cognitive function in men from the United States [22]. Converse to our study, the authors found LoC to be very stable over time (LoC changed over an 18-year follow-up period in 30% of our study participants). Similar to our study however, they reported cognitive function to be highest among men who consistently demonstrated an internal locus of control over time, and lowest among those who demonstrated an external propensity over time. Our findings suggest that associations are similar in women and that they can be observed as early as age ~50 years. In addition to this, another study reported that mothers with an internal prenatal LoC, on average, had offspring with a higher IQ at age 8 years [34], suggesting that LoC may be associated with cognitive function across the entire life course; not just in mid-late adulthood.

Strengths and limitations

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to assess associations between LoC measures and cognitive function in women from the general population. LoC was measured using a validated questionnaire [35] at two time-points, meaning we were able to examine how change in LoC relates to our outcome of interest. We also had data for a variety of cognitive tests, enabling us to assess the effect of LoC on different cognitive domains and also on overall cognitive functioning. One limitation of our study is the possibility of selection bias, as 41% of the people who attended the follow-up clinic where the cognitive tests were conducted did not have data for one or more measures of LoC and/or one or more potential confounders. To examine the possibility of selection bias, we examined whether the unadjusted associations were similar in a larger sample of participants with only a measure of LoC at the first-time point and a measure of cognitive function (n=2241 compared to n=1178 in our main analyses). Results were very similar, suggesting that selection bias is unlikely to fully explain our findings. It is also worth noting that people with an external locus of control and with lower cognitive function scores are more likely to be lost to follow-up, meaning any bias due to selection is likely to be towards the null. Finally, we only studied British women, thus we cannot assume that our results would generalise to men, or women from different national or ethnic backgrounds.

Conclusion

Our findings suggest that an external LoC is detrimental to cognitive function. LoC can change over time, and interventions to increase internality may help to minimise the adverse consequences on cognitive health later in life. Further longitudinal studies should examine whether LoC is associated with cognitive function across the whole life course and with cognitive decline over time. Genetic studies may be able to identify variants associated with LoC, which would pave the way for examining whether or not these associations are causal, using methods such as Mendelian randomization.

Materials and Methods

Study population

The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) is a prospective birth cohort study from southwest England that recruited 14,541 pregnant women, resident in 3 Bristol-based health districts, with an expected date of delivery between April 1991 and December 1992 [43] . Our analysis uses data from the mothers in this cohort [36]. The study website contains details of all available data through a fully searchable data dictionary and variable search tool (www.bris.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/our-data/). Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee and the Local Research Ethics Committees. Approximately 25 years after recruitment into the cohort, women were invited to attend a follow-up research clinic at which cognitive function was assessed. A total of 2893 woman attended this clinic (mean age 51 years, standard deviation 4.4 years).

Assessing Locus of Control

Women completed a condensed version of the Adult Nowicki Strickland Internal External control scale (ANSIE) [35] in questionnaires administered at mean ages 30 and 48 years. The original ANSIE comprises 40 items in a yes/no format, which assess perceived control. The version used in the present study comprises 12 of the original 40 items, which were chosen after factor analysis of the ANSIE administered as a pilot to 135 mothers [34]. The 12 questions loaded onto a single factor of general LoC. The 12 questions used are shown elsewhere (Golding et al., 2017). From the responses, a ‘LoC score’ was derived; the higher the score the more external the LoC. Scores ranged from 0 to 12 and approximated a normal distribution, with a median of 3 at both measurement occasions.

Assessing cognitive function in mid-adulthood

A series of tests were conducted at a follow-up research clinic at mean age 51 years to assess different domains of cognitive function including verbal fluency, short-term and prospective memory and processing speed. All cognitive function outcomes measured in this study are associated with mortality [37]. Cognitive function was assessed with verbal fluency [38], logical memory [39], delayed logical memory [39], digit backwards [40], digit symbol coding [40], and spot the word tests [41]. Full assessment details of each cognitive test are provided in the online Supplement.

Covariables

We considered social class and ethnicity to be potential confounders of the association between LoC and cognitive function in mid-adulthood. Participants’ social class was indicated by both head of household social class and educational attainment. Participants’ SEP in adulthood was reported at enrolment into the study (during years 1991-1992) as the highest of own and partner’s occupational class groups using the 1991 British Office of Population and Census Statistics (OPCS) classification. It was coded as ‘professional’, ‘managerial and technical’, ‘skilled non-manual’, ‘skilled manual’ and ‘partly or unskilled manual’. Women reported their ethnicity in questionnaires administered at enrolment. Age at the time of outcome assessment was recorded.

Statistical analysis

Locus of control measures

We examined associations using both a continuous and categorical measure of LoC at mean ages 30 and 48 years. This enabled us to examine whether associations exist across the whole spectrum of LoC or whether there is likely a threshold effect. The continuous scores ranged from 0 to 12 and were normally distributed with a mean (standard deviation, SD) of 3.3 (1.9) at age 30 years and 3.0 (1.9) at age 48 years. The higher the score the more external the locus of control. The categorical measures classify participants as having an internal or external LoC at each time-point, with external being defined as above the median score of 3 (on both measurement occasions), and internal equal to or lower than the median score. Finally, we generated a measure of change in LoC across the two time-points which consisted of four categories: (1) external at both time-points, (2) internal at both time-points, (3) internal to external and (4) external to internal.

Generating composite scores of cognitive function

In addition to assessing individual cognitive function tests which reflect different underlying systems (e.g. fluid vs crystallised intelligence), composite scores of cognitive function were also created using the method devised by Guralnik et al [42]. Combining measures into a composite score may identify a much higher risk group (i.e. participants doing very badly on all tests), thus allowing us to assess the extremes of cognitive performance, which may be more revealing in a middle-aged population that is generally functioning well. Each cognitive function test score was rescaled to lie between 0 and 1, giving all measures equal weight in the final composite scores (see online Supplement for further details of the rescaling procedure). Participants unable to perform a test were assigned a value of 0. Rescaled cognitive function measures were summed to create normally distributed aggregate cognitive function score with a range of 0 to 6.

Examining associations between LoC and cognitive function

We examined associations of the continuous LoC scores, the categorical LoC measure and the change in LoC measure with the composite measure of overall cognitive function. All associations were examined in the following models: 1) unadjusted, 2) adjusted for educational attainment, head of household social class, ethnicity and age at outcome assessment and 3) additionally adjusting for the previous measure of LoC (except for where change in LoC is the exposure).

Eligibility criteria and missing data

Participants were eligible to be included in analyses if they had data for all variables included in the analyses (i.e. all six cognitive function tests, measures of LoC at mean ages 30 and 48 years and all potential confounders, n=1178). A total of 1779 women were excluded from these analyses due to missing data for one or more of these variables. To investigate potential selection bias due to missing data, we examined whether unadjusted associations were similar in the larger sample of participants with a measure of LoC at the first-time point only (i.e. at mean age 30 years, n=2241).

Additional analyses

Firstly, we examined associations of the continuous LoC scores and change in LoC with the six individual cognitive tests, to establish whether LoC (or change in LoC) is particularly strongly associated with certain cognitive domains. Secondly, we considered that exposure to psychosocial adversity (for example, physical or emotional abuse and neglect) in childhood may be a potential confounder of the association between LoC and cognitive function. However, only 79% of participants additionally had a measure of exposure to psychosocial adversity in childhood. Thus, adjusting for this in our main analysis would have greatly reduced our sample size (and thereby statistical power). To examine whether associations were likely to be confounded by exposure to psychosocial adversity, we assessed whether associations between LoC and cognitive function were similar after additional adjustment for psychosocial adversity in the subgroup of participants with data for this variable (n=929). Details of the measure of psychosocial adversity are provided in the online Supplement.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary File

Acknowledgments

We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists and nurses.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors have no conflict of interests to declare.

Funding

The UK Medical Research Council and Wellcome (Grant ref: 102215/2/13/2) and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. This publication is the work of the authors and will serve as guarantors for the contents of this paper. A comprehensive list of grants funding is available on the ALSPAC website (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/external/documents/grant-acknowledgements.pdf). ELA is funded by an MRC fellowship (ref: MR/P014437/1). The research programme on LoC in which JG and SN are involved was specifically funded by a grant from the John Templeton.

References

  • 1. Nowicki SJr, Bonner J, Feather B. Effects of locus of control and differential analogue interview procedures on the perceived therapeutic relationship. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1972; 38:434–38. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0032903 [PubMed]
  • 2. Garaigordobil M, Bernarás E, Jaureguizar J, Machimbarrena JM. Childhood Depression: Relation to Adaptive, Clinical and Predictor Variables. Front Psychol. 2017; 8:821. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00821 [PubMed]
  • 3. Bjørkløf GH, Engedal K, Selbæk G, Maia DB, Borza T, Benth JS, Helvik AS. Can depression in psychogeriatric inpatients at one year follow-up be explained by locus of control and coping strategies? Aging Ment Health. 2018; 22:379–88. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2016.1262817 [PubMed]
  • 4. Moshki M, Baloochi Beydokhti T, Cheravi K. The effect of educational intervention on prevention of postpartum depression: an application of health locus of control. J Clin Nurs. 2014; 23:2256–63. https://doi.org/10.1111/jocn.12505 [PubMed]
  • 5. Culpin I, Stapinski L, Miles OB, Araya R, Joinson C. Exposure to socioeconomic adversity in early life and risk of depression at 18 years: the mediating role of locus of control. J Affect Disord. 2015; 183:269–78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.05.030 [PubMed]
  • 6. Hoehn-Saric R, McLeod DR. Locus of control in chronic anxiety disorders. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1985; 72:529–35. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1985.tb02650.x [PubMed]
  • 7. Neymotin F, Nemzer LR. Locus of control and obesity. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne). 2014; 5:159. https://doi.org/10.3389/fendo.2014.00159 [PubMed]
  • 8. Wineman NM. Obesity: locus of control, body image, weight loss, and age-at-onset. Nurs Res. 1980; 29:231–37. https://doi.org/10.1097/00006199-198007000-00015 [PubMed]
  • 9. Holt CL, Roth DL, Huang J, Clark EM. Gender differences in the roles of religion and locus of control on alcohol use and smoking among African Americans. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2015; 76:482–92. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsad.2015.76.482 [PubMed]
  • 10. Lindqvist R, Aberg H. Locus of control in relation to smoking cessation during pregnancy. Scand J Public Health. 2002; 30:30–35. https://doi.org/10.1177/14034948020300010801 [PubMed]
  • 11. Rosenbaum M, Argon S. Locus of control and success in self-initiated attempts to stop smoking. J Clin Psychol. 1979; 35:870–72. https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-4679(197910)35:4<870::AID-JCLP2270350437>3.0.CO;2-D [PubMed]
  • 12. Wallston BD, Wallston KA. Locus of control and health: a review of the literature. Health Educ Monogr. 1978; 6:107–17. https://doi.org/10.1177/109019817800600102 [PubMed]
  • 13. Smith RE. Changes in locus of control as a function of life crisis resolution. J Abnorm Psychol. 1970; 75:329–32. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0029336 [PubMed]
  • 14. Zhang W, Liu H, Jiang X, Wu D, Tian Y. A longitudinal study of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms and its relationship with coping skill and locus of control in adolescents after an earthquake in China. PLoS One. 2014; 9:e88263. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088263 [PubMed]
  • 15. Diamond MJ, Shapiro JL. Changes in locus of control as a function of encounter group experiences: a study and replication. J Abnorm Psychol. 1973; 82:514–18. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0035383 [PubMed]
  • 16. Wolinsky FD, Vander Weg MW, Martin R, Unverzagt FW, Willis SL, Marsiske M, Rebok GW, Morris JN, Ball KK, Tennstedt SL. Does cognitive training improve internal locus of control among older adults? J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2010; 65:591–98. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbp117 [PubMed]
  • 17. Sharp C, Hurford DP, Allison J, Sparks R, Cameron BP. Facilitation of internal locus of control in adolescent alcoholics through a brief biofeedback-assisted autogenic relaxation training procedure. J Subst Abuse Treat. 1997; 14:55–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0740-5472(96)00127-4 [PubMed]
  • 18. Bielak AA, Hultsch DF, Levy-Ajzenkopf J, MacDonald SW, Hunter MA, Strauss E. Short-term changes in general and memory-specific control beliefs and their relationship to cognition in younger and older adults. Int J Aging Hum Dev. 2007; 65:53–71. https://doi.org/10.2190/G458-X101-0338-746X [PubMed]
  • 19. Soederberg Miller LM, Lachman ME. Cognitive Performance and the Role of Control Beliefs in Midlife. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2000; 7:69–85. https://doi.org/10.1076/1382-5585(200006)7:2;1-U;FT069
  • 20. Neupert SD, Allaire JC. I think I can, I think I can: examining the within-person coupling of control beliefs and cognition in older adults. Psychol Aging. 2012; 27:742–49. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026447 [PubMed]
  • 21. Brown BR, Granick S. Cognitive and psychosocial differences between I and E locus of control aged persons. Exp Aging Res. 1983; 9:107–10. https://doi.org/10.1080/03610738308258435 [PubMed]
  • 22. Wight RG, Aneshensel CS, Seeman M, Seeman TE. Late life cognition among men: a life course perspective on psychosocial experience. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2003; 37:173–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-4943(03)00046-3 [PubMed]
  • 23. Sabia S, Dugravot A, Dartigues JF, Abell J, Elbaz A, Kivimäki M, Singh-Manoux A. Physical activity, cognitive decline, and risk of dementia: 28 year follow-up of Whitehall II cohort study. BMJ. 2017; 357:j2709. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.j2709 [PubMed]
  • 24. Osler M, Christensen GT, Garde E, Mortensen EL, Christensen K. Cognitive ability in young adulthood and risk of dementia in a cohort of Danish men, brothers, and twins. Alzheimers Dement. 2017; 13:1355–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2017.04.003 [PubMed]
  • 25. Nowicki S, Ellis G, Iles-Caven Y, Gregory S, Golding J. Events associated with stability and change in adult locus of control orientation over a six-year period. Pers Individ Dif. 2018; 126:85–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.01.017 [PubMed]
  • 26. Agüero-Torres H, Fratiglioni L, Guo Z, Viitanen M, von Strauss E, Winblad B. Dementia is the major cause of functional dependence in the elderly: 3-year follow-up data from a population-based study. Am J Public Health. 1998; 88:1452–56. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.88.10.1452 [PubMed]
  • 27. Watson NL, Rosano C, Boudreau RM, Simonsick EM, Ferrucci L, Sutton-Tyrrell K, Hardy SE, Atkinson HH, Yaffe K, Satterfield S, Harris TB, Newman AB, Health AB, and Health ABC Study. Executive function, memory, and gait speed decline in well-functioning older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2010; 65:1093–100. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/glq111 [PubMed]
  • 28. Lowry KA, Vallejo AN, Studenski SA. Successful aging as a continuum of functional independence: lessons from physical disability models of aging. Aging Dis. 2012; 3:5–15. [PubMed]
  • 29. Batty GD, Deary IJ, Zaninotto P. Association of Cognitive Function With Cause-Specific Mortality in Middle and Older Age: Follow-up of Participants in the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Am J Epidemiol. 2016; 183:183–90. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwv139 [PubMed]
  • 30. Gillum RF, Obisesan TO. Physical activity, cognitive function, and mortality in a US national cohort. Ann Epidemiol. 2010; 20:251–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.annepidem.2010.01.005 [PubMed]
  • 31. Gale CR, Martyn CN, Cooper C. Cognitive impairment and mortality in a cohort of elderly people. BMJ. 1996; 312:608–11. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.312.7031.608 [PubMed]
  • 32. Pavlik VN, de Moraes SA, Szklo M, Knopman DS, Mosley THJr, Hyman DJ. Relation between cognitive function and mortality in middle-aged adults: the atherosclerosis risk in communities study. Am J Epidemiol. 2003; 157:327–34. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwf209 [PubMed]
  • 33. Nowicki S, Iles-Caven Y, Gregory S, Ellis G, Golding J. The Impact of Prenatal Parental Locus of Control on Children’s Psychological Outcomes in Infancy and Early Childhood: A Prospective 5 Year Study. Front Psychol. 2017; 8:546. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00546 [PubMed]
  • 34. Golding J, Gregory S, Ellis GL, Iles-Caven Y, Nowicki S. Prenatal Internal Locus of Control Is Positively Associated with Offspring IQ, Mediated through Parenting Behavior, Prenatal Lifestyle and Social Circumstances. Front Psychol. 2017; 8:1429. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01429 [PubMed]
  • 35. Nowicki SJr, Duke MP. A Locus of Control Scale for Noncollege as Well as College Adults. J Pers Assess. 1974; 38:136–37. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.1974.10119950
  • 36. Fraser A, Macdonald-Wallis C, Tilling K, Boyd A, Golding J, Davey Smith G, Henderson J, Macleod J, Molloy L, Ness A, Ring S, Nelson SM, Lawlor DA. Cohort Profile: The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. Int J Epidemiol. 2013; 42:97–110. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dys066 [PubMed]
  • 37. Small BJ, Bäckman L. Cognitive correlates of mortality: evidence from a population-based sample of very old adults. Psychol Aging. 1997; 12:309–13. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.12.2.309 [PubMed]
  • 38. Lezak M, Howieson D, Loring D, Hannay H, Fischer J. 2004. Neuropsychological assessment. (New York, US: Oxford Univeristy Press).
  • 39. Wechsler D. 1998. Wechsler Memory Scale: Administration and scoring manual. (London: The Psychological Corporation).
  • 40. Wechsler D. 1998. WAIS-IIIUK administration and scoring manual. (London: The Psychological Corporation).
  • 41. Baddeley A, Emslie H, Nimmo-Smith I. The Spot-the-Word test: a robust estimate of verbal intelligence based on lexical decision. Br J Clin Psychol. 1993; 32:55–65. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8260.1993.tb01027.x [PubMed]
  • 42. Guralnik JM, Butterworth S, Wadsworth ME, Kuh D. Childhood socioeconomic status predicts physical functioning a half century later. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2006; 61:694–701. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/61.7.694 [PubMed]
  • 43. Boyd A, Golding J, Macleod J, Lawlor DA, Fraser A, Henderson J, Molloy L, Ness A, Ring S, Davey SG . Cohort Profile: the 'children of the 90s'--the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. J Int J Epidemiol. 2013; 42:111–27. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dys064 [PubMed]